In 4-5 pages, (Time Roman, 12-font, double-spaced), address three (3) questions.
- What is the role of teachers in helping children and teen’s development?
- What is the influence of peer groups on children and teens?
- What is the influence of family on children and teens?
No abstract is necessary. APA in-text citation and reference list required.
Belonging to and exclusion from the
peer group in schools: influences on
adolescents’ moral choices
Luba Falk Feigenberg*a, Melissa Steel Kinga, Dennis J. Barra,b
and Robert L. Selmana
a Harvard University, USA; bFacing History and Ourselves, Boston, USA
This paper reports on a mixed methods study of adolescents’ responses to case material about
social exclusion. First, a qualitative coding method is presented that describes the way adolescents
choose and justify strategies to negotiate such situations. The responses were then analysed
quantitatively using chi square tests and multinomial logistic regression. Findings indicate that
adolescents’ interpretation of their social context was a significant factor in their choice of strategy.
Those adolescents who invoked normative rules and conventions as the most salient justifications
were more likely to recommend bystanding rather than joining in the exclusion. However,
adolescents who viewed the protagonist’s own choice as an opportunity for making long-lasting
positive changes in the social environment were more likely to recommend helping the victim.
Gender and school context also were associated with adolescents’ choice of strategy. Implications
for research in moral development as well as practical implications for school-based programming
are discussed.
Introduction: the problem of peer group social exclusion and bullying in the
schools
In her ninth grade class on ethics and history, Eve Shalen wrote an essay about her
involvement with an incident of social exclusion and ostracism that occurred in early
adolescence. About her experience with social relations in middle school, she says:
My eighth grade consisted of 28 students most of whom knew each other from the age
of five or six. Although we grew up together, we still had class outcasts. From second
grade on, a small élite group spent a large portion of their time harassing two or three of
the others. I was one of those two or three, though I don’t know why…The harassment
was subtle. It came in the form of muffled giggles when I talked and rolled eyes when I
turned around. If I was out in the playground and approached a group of people, they
*Corresponding author. Harvard Graduate School of Education, Larsen Hall 610, Appian Way,
Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. Email: [email protected]
Journal of Moral Education
Vol. 37, No. 2, June 2008, pp. 165–184
ISSN 0305-7240 (print)/ISSN 1465-3877 (online)/08/020165-20
# 2008 Journal of Moral Education Ltd
DOI: 10.1080/03057240802009306
often fell silent. Sometimes someone would not see me coming and I would catch the
tail end of a joke at my expense.
There was another girl in our class who was perhaps even more rejected than I. One
day during lunch…one of the popular girls in the class came up to me to show me
something she said I wouldn’t want to miss. We walked to a corner of the playground
where a group of three or four sat. One of them read aloud from a small book, which I
was told was the girl’s diary. I sat down and, laughing till my sides hurt, heard my voice
finally blend with the others. Looking back, I wonder how I could have participated in
mocking this girl when I knew perfectly well what it felt like to be mocked myself. I
would like to say that if I were in that situation today I would react differently, but I
can’t honestly be sure. (Facing History and Ourselves, 1994, pp. 29–30).
Experiences of social exclusion such as this are quite common to early adolescents
who struggle to navigate social relationships every day. Much of the discourse on
social exclusion focuses on why adolescents who may ‘know better’ still join in when
they see their peers mocking or harassing another student (Bosworth et al., 1999;
Nansel et al., 2001). However, there is little research that examines what might help
adolescents ‘think beyond’ not joining in and, instead, influence them to get
involved to help the victim. In other words, what factors might influence adolescents
to choose to stand up for themselves or others?
In this paper, we explore the way early adolescents think about the choice to
bystand, join the perpetrators or defend the victim in a situation of social exclusion,
such as the one Eve Shalen describes in her school. First, we review the research
literature on how various factors—social cognitive, cultural and contextual—influence
adolescents’ choices about difficult social actions and decisions. We then describe the
construction and validation of a framework to classify both the range of strategies
adolescents recommend in response to the In Group Assessment, a qualitative
measure based on the Eve Shalen case material, as well as the justifications they give
for their choice (Barr, 2005). Next, we present findings about the influence of school
contextual factors on the variation in social choices. We conclude with a discussion of
the implications of our findings for research and practice in moral development and
educational programming to reduce social exclusion.
Social exclusion: definitions of the problem
Often defined as a physical, verbal or psychological action intended to cause fear,
distress or harm to the victim, social exclusion during childhood and adolescence
occurs within the context of an asymmetric power relationship, where a more
powerful child, or group of children, oppresses the less powerful one(s) (Olweus,
1993). Social exclusion in adolescence often includes ostracism, teasing, harassment
and bullying (Swain, 1998). Research in the US suggests between 5 and 27% of
adolescents admit to having excluded a peer (Nansel et al., 2001; Dake et al., 2003).
It is now recognised that social exclusion is seldom the action of only one
individual. The group dynamics required for and created by such behaviour
contributes to the overall culture and climate of schools and social groups. Not
surprisingly, schools with higher rates of exclusion are perceived as less safe (Astor
et al., 2002; Dupper & Meyer-Adams, 2002). Faced with such statistics, schools
166 L. F. Feigenberg et al.
struggle to find effective responses to social exclusion and to prevent its negative
consequences in their hallways, cafeterias, gyms and classes.
Further, rigid rules and inflexible consequences in schools typically address the
behaviour of those identified as the ‘perpetrators’ but not that of the ‘bystanders’—
those students who either passively watch or actively incite the exclusion (Staub, 2002).
In fact, even though most adolescents believe social exclusion is wrong, they often do
not try to intervene and usually stand by passively (Tisak et al., 1997; O’Connell et al.,
1999). In order to address this problem, it is important to understand what may cause
adolescents to make choices about their own behaviour in these contexts.
Theoretical orientations and empirical evidence: a brief review
Psychological theories that focus on child and adolescent social cognitive
development primarily describe the quality of individuals’ thought processes that
relate to or influence their social behaviour. Social information processing models,
for example, portray the cognitive steps necessary for individuals to make decisions
about social action (Dodge, 1986; Dodge & Price, 1994; Crick & Dodge, 1996).
Adolescents engage in a series of thought processes and ultimately choose an action
from a range of perceived possibilities. Antisocial behaviour, or social exclusion, may
result from misperceptions of the actions and intentions of others, a deficiency that
may occur at any point in the social information process (Fontaine et al., 2002).
According to these models, adolescents’ choices are due to internal cognitive
structures and abilities where the selection of exclusionary behaviour may be an
indicator of inaccurate or distorted social perceptions (Camodeca et al., 2003).
Other cognitive developmental approaches examine adolescents’ thought processes
as related to social interactions or the understanding, negotiation and meaning of
social relationships over time (Selman, 2003). This approach focuses specifically on
the conditions under which children develop and use the ability to coordinate different
social perspectives (Collins, 2002), rather than conceptualising social competence as a
sequence of social cognitive information processing steps (Selman, 1980; Keller &
Edelstein, 1991). For example, adolescents who have more difficulty coordinating
their own and others’ points of view are at greater risk for peer conflict and may be
more likely to participate in social exclusion (Selman et al., 1992, 1997). Like social
informational processing models, however, many earlier social cognitive-develop-
mental theories (Kohlberg, 1971) located the impetus for adolescents’ choice of social
action primarily in the minds of individuals or at the individual level of analysis. By
focusing mainly on individuals’ social cognitions, these models often do not consider
how other factors may influence adolescents’ social choices, especially under
challenging, complex or ambiguous conditions (Steinberg, 2003).
Differences between adolescents’ social viewpoints and actions can also be attributed
to population level or cultural factors. Research on individuals’ membership of broad
social systems suggests that group affiliation, such as gender, race or socioeconomic
status, may shape the way they think about their choices about behaviour. For example,
gender has been shown to be an important influence on social exclusion. When faced
Moral choices 167
with such situations, early adolescent boys tend to prefer to join in with the
perpetrators, while girls tend to side with the victim (Nansel et al., 2001; Seals &
Young, 2003). In contrast, research that focuses specifically on social aggression—
defined as the manipulation of a relationship in order to damage ‘another’s self-esteem,
social status or both’ (Underwood, 2003, p. 23)—suggests girls are much more likely
than boys to be the perpetrators of socially aggressive acts (Crick et al., 1996; Galen &
Underwood, 1997). Further, girls are expected, by both boys and girls, to be the
perpetrators of socially aggressive acts (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Lastly, there appear
to be gender differences in preferred coping strategies and desire for adult intervention
in situations of social exclusion (Naylor et al., 2001; Gamliel et al., 2003).
While gender plays an important role in adolescents’ behaviour in social exclusion,
such behaviour does not appear to vary by race or ethnicity, at least in the US (Leff et
al., 1999; Seals & Young, 2003). Yet, when cultural characteristics are considered as
part of the larger context within which exclusion occurs, adolescents view race, as well
as gender, as important factors in thinking about when such behaviour may be
appropriate. For example, adolescents consider others’ racial background as a valid
reason for excluding individuals from particular social relationships or situations, such
as school-based clubs or peer groups (Killen & Stangor, 2001; Killen et al., 2002).
The roles individuals assume within the peer group context also affect the way
they think about social choices. Individuals tend to take on particular roles, across
both situations and time, that contribute to the structural components of intergroup
dynamics. In particular, adolescents tend to view their moral choices differently
depending on whether they are witness to an event or a perpetrator (Salmivalli et al.,
1996; Sutton & Smith, 1999). In other words, adolescents’ role in the social context
influences their choices for behaviour. In this sense, context extends beyond the
impact of external social structures and systems on social and relational behaviour.
Rather, context can be thought of as the system of ‘social activities and cultural
meanings’ in which an individual participates (Serpell, 2002). The way students
understand their choices, then, is grounded in their context and social experiences
(Burton et al., 1996).
The school context, for one, plays an especially important role in the way students
perceive risk behaviour (Kuperminc et al., 1997). For example, the level of safety
adolescents perceive in their schools and communities is related to their thinking
about the meaning of threatening behaviour (Espelage et al., 2000). When students
believe their social environment is unsafe they are more likely to interpret others’
behaviour as hostile or aggressive. The climate fostered in the school plays an
important role in students’ choices around peer group actions (Salmivalli & Voeten,
2004), such as whether they join the perpetrator(s) or defend the victim.
Research questions
This study is guided by the following research questions:
1. What strategies do students recommend for negotiating a situation of social
exclusion and what justifications do they offer for their chosen strategy?
168 L. F. Feigenberg et al.
2. Do students’ recommended strategies and justifications for negotiating a
situation of social exclusion differ by their role in the social context, i.e.
whether they are asked to assume the role of witness or perpetrator?
3. Do students’ recommended strategies for negotiating a situation of social
exclusion vary as a function of their perceptions of the social context, i.e.
school or classroom climate?
To address these questions, we designed a mixed-methods study. First, we explored
students’ open-ended responses to case material about a situation of social exclusion
for the strategies they recommend and the justifications they offer for their choices.
Next, we translated the qualitative codes into quantitative categories in order to
examine the influences on students’ choice of strategy. We interpret the implications
of our findings for research in social development as well as school-based social
development programming.
Methods
The research context and procedure
The research described in this paper is part of an ongoing collaborative project with
Facing History and Ourselves (hereafter Facing History),1 an international
organisation that provides resources for teachers and a program for students that
focuses on human behaviour and ethics as a bridge between history and the self
(Tollefson et al., 2004).
Schools with teachers who were known to use the Facing History program and
who were willing to be included in a quasi-experimental study were recruited to
participate in this study. A second school was then recruited within the same town
that matched for similar characteristics. All students in the selected classrooms were
eligible to participate in this research. Only students who returned written parental
consent forms were included in the study; participation rates were above 75% across
the classrooms. Measures were administered during class time by the teachers in the
study and were then turned over to members of the research team. No identifying
information was collected and students’ confidentiality was guaranteed. The
students completed the measures before they had any contact with Facing History
materials and so the data are not construed as evaluation outcome data.
Setting and participants
The participants in this study were students in five public middle schools in
Massachusetts. The schools were all located in districts with a predominantly white,
middle-class student population. As Table 1 shows, there is some variation in
socioeconomic and educational characteristics across the schools, as suggested by
the percentage of students who qualify for free or reduced price lunch (information
about the students in this study and the schools they attended was provided by the
Facing History organisation).
Moral choices 169
The sample for this study includes 168 students in five Grade 8 classrooms, one
from each school in the study. One hundred and four girls and 64 boys participated.
Students ranged in age from 12–13 years old. No other individual-level demographic
information is available.
Measures
After reading the case material about Eve Shalen, participants completed the In
Group Assessment. This is a survey measure, with two primary questions based on
the case study of Eve Shalen and seven follow-up open-ended items. The measure
was designed to challenge students to assume different vantage points, such as
bystander or victim, about a situation of social exclusion, as well as to capture their
thinking about the issues of interpersonal relationships and social exclusion more
generally (Barr, 2005). The content and language of the assessment questions rely
on the discourse used in Facing History, while the structure of the questions is
driven by a psychological theory about how children develop the capacity to
coordinate various social perspectives, which is central to their understanding and
negotiation of social relationships as well as their developing social awareness
(Selman et al., 1997; Selman, 2003).
For the purposes of this study, we analysed students’ responses to two questions
on the In Group Assessment. The first question asked students to respond as a
witness or bystander to the social exclusion: 1(a) List at least two different ways that
Eve could have acted when she witnessed her classmates picking on other students.
(b) Which would be the best way? (c) Why would that be the best way?
In the second question, students were asked to suggest choices when invited to
join the perpetrators: 2(a) List at least two things Eve could have done when she was
invited to join in the teasing of the other girl. (b) Which would be the best way? (c)
Why would that be the best way?
In each of the two questions, students were asked to write about the choices they
perceived for negotiating the situation and their justifications for each choice. This
allowed us to compare strategies and justifications across each of the two vantage
points. Data for this study include all students who complied with the instructions
for both questions.
Table 1. Sample demographics (n5168)
School n Race (% white of the total
population)1 % Eligible for free/reduced
price lunch1
A 34 81.0 9.6
B 63 92.0 4.8
C 19 88.0 7.1
D 14 80.0 30.5
E 38 80.0 27.7
Notes: 1Massachusetts Department of Education (2004)
170 L. F. Feigenberg et al.
Analysis plan
Qualitative analysis: coding development
To address the first research question, we initially examined the responses to code
for the strategies students suggested for negotiating the choice to witness or join in
the social exclusion. Using a semi-grounded approach (Glaser & Strauss, 1967), we
allowed the data, rather than theory, to guide these analyses. First, we open-coded
the responses by grouping similar strategies together. We then clustered the
suggested strategies by salient themes, which resulted in three strategy categories.
The codebook for these strategy categories, including exemplars of each, is shown in
Table 2.
To examine the second portion of the responses, where students justify why their
chosen strategy would be the best one for the situation, we focused on the factors in
the social environment students perceive to be most salient when choosing a
strategy. We used an inductive approach (Boyatzis, 1998) to allow both the data and
theory to guide the coding. This analysis involved a greater amount of interpretation
on our part than the analysis for strategy codes, as we attempted to capture key
elements about how the strategy was explained and the reason given for its selection.
Table 3 provides the codebook for the four justification categories as well as
examples of each.
A three-person team coded the data. Each phase of coding began with a discussion
of the codebooks for strategy and justification (as shown in Tables 2 and 3), the
establishment of anchor responses and independent scoring until an acceptable level
of inter-rater agreement was reached. Both the strategy and justification categories
had over 92% direct agreement and Kappas greater than .9 (Bakeman & Quera
Table 2. Codebook for strategy categories
Code description Anchor response
Upstand N Requires intervention in the existing
situation of ostracism
‘She could of told them to stop, and
that they were being mean.’
N Articulates an action that assists the
victim, such as standing up to the group
or comforting the victim
N Aligns against the mocking of the girl
‘She should make friends with that
girl.’
‘She should of just told a teacher what
was going on.’
Perpetrate N Aligns with the group mocking the girl ‘She could play along and make fun
of the kids too.’
‘She should go with the girls.’
‘Ignore them and just go on with her
business.’
‘Make an excuse and walk away.’
N Action implies that the invitation to join
the mocking has been accepted
N Contributes to the existing situation of
ostracism
Bystand N Aligns with neither the victim nor the
group doing the mocking
N Avoids involvement with the existing
situation of ostracism
N Uninvolvement, active as in walking away,
or passive, by minding one’s own business
Moral choices 171
[1995] suggest .6 to .8 to be adequate and above .8 to be excellent). Approximately
20% of the sample was used during training and reliability procedures. Once the
training was completed, the remaining surveys were divided among the raters and
scored independently. All of the surveys were used in subsequent analyses.
Quantitative analysis
In order to examine the relationships between strategy and justification categories,
we created categorical variables and assigned each code a numerical value (0–2 for
Table 3. Codebook for justification categories
Code description Anchor response
Conventional N References social norms, conventions or
rules (formal or informal) as the main
guiding principle
‘It is the right thing to do.’
‘It would be easier.’
‘Because I think it’s the best.’
N Highlights the efficiency or expediency
of the recommended strategy
‘It would keep everyone out
of trouble.’
N Does not explicate reasoning beyond
simple explanations of cost-benefit
analyses that imply one action is simply
‘better’ than another
Safety N Indicates protection as a priority ‘So they don’t start picking
on her too.’
‘That way nothing bad
happens to the victim.’
N Perceives an immediate threat to one’s
emotional or physical well-being
N Indicates that the main goal is to stop
the current situation of ostracism ‘To make sure she doesn’t get
hurt.’N Does not reference long term
consequences or implications of
recommended strategy
Relational N Highlights the formation or maintenance
of interpersonal relationship(s)
‘And I could be considered
the ‘‘Popular’’ girl.’
N Articulates desire for belonging or
connectedness with another person or
with a group of people
‘Because she’d feel like she fit
in.’
‘Because she knows what it
feels like.’N Identifies a connection between people’s
experiences or emotions
Prosocial
Transformational
N Explains connections between the
recommended action and possible future
consequences or implications
‘They might realize they’re
doing the wrong thing and
not do it again.’
N Speculates about the possible
development of or changes in other
people’s thinking or beliefs
‘I believe that if enough
people are willing to do
something about a
problem, the problem
would not exist anymore.’
‘She would make a good
influence on other people.’
N Articulates opportunities for group
dynamics to shift as a result of the
recommended action
N Implies that the recommended action
coul